News! Articolo pubblicato: Malattie fisiche psichiche - EFFETTO NOCEBO Click here

Home


Syn­op­sis Body and Sense (After Psy­cho­so­mat­ics) [1]


Why does the mind body prob­lem, the so-​called MBP, ac­com­pa­ny the whole path of West­ern thought? 

Be­cause it is an end­less topic that be­gins with the re­la­tion­ship be­tween soul and body, a crux that refers to an in­fi­nite se­ries of para­mount ques­tions that lie well be­yond the mind body re­la­tion­ship. 

More­over, no one has ever solved the un­der­ly­ing issue of the prob­lem. 

Many have tried. 

Men­tal oc­cur­rences, as some hy­poth­e­size, may only be some­thing that takes place in­side us, in our body, or at the very least in tight con­nec­tion to bod­i­ly oc­cur­rences.  

While oth­ers hold that mind and body are iden­ti­cal and bear the same sub­stance, only ap­pear­ance dif­fers. Or on the con­trary, the two process­es, or­gan­ic and men­tal, are deemed com­plete­ly dif­fer­ent, de­spite oc­cur­ring to­geth­er all along.  

We are faced with a pletho­ra of hy­pothe­ses that chase one an­oth­er with­out ever al­low­ing to reach a so­lu­tion.   

Let us con­sid­er the cur­rent main­stream hy­poth­e­sis, the so-​called on­to­log­i­cal monism with du­al­ism of knowl­edge: bod­i­ly and men­tal phe­nom­e­na are only con­cep­tu­al­ly dis­tinct, in thought and in speech, be­cause we al­ways find them to­geth­er in ex­ist­ing re­al­i­ty, pre­cise­ly as if they be­longed to an an­cient unit (the mind-​body unit, with a hy­phen). 

Come to think of it, what is this unit? Where in the world is this pre­sumed sin­gle ob­ject that West­ern thought might have split in two? What does it mean? One can but an­swer that this is un­think­able.  

Fur­ther­more, sup­pos­ing we man­aged to truly find the real cor­re­spon­dence be­tween men­tal and bod­i­ly phe­nom­e­na, which type of cor­re­spon­dence would it be? Be­cause we are well aware that the re­la­tion­ship be­tween a phys­i­cal state and a men­tal state is nei­ther con­stant, nor sim­ple.

Those who seem to have solved the un­der­ly­ing issue of the MBP:

Pro­fes­sor and re­search di­rec­tor at the Dern­er In­sti­tute of Adel­phi Uni­ver­si­ty in New York, Bucci is a psy­cho­an­a­lyst who has un­der­tak­en a par­tic­u­lar­ly dense re­search ca­reer to broad­en the hori­zons of Freudi­an psy­cho­analy­sis to­wards the con­tents of neu­ro­science, on one hand, and of cog­ni­tive sci­ence on the other (1997, Psy­cho­analy­sis and Cog­ni­tive Sci­ence, NY: The Guil­ford Press).  

Her model, called ‘mul­ti­ple code the­o­ry’, on one hand in­deed ap­pears as a in-​depth analy­sis of the Freudi­an dif­fer­en­ti­a­tion be­tween pri­ma­ry and sec­ondary process­es, while at the same time marked­ly dis­tanc­ing it­self from the lat­ter. Bucci holds that the con­cepts of pri­ma­ry and sec­ondary process­es, de­spite their im­por­tance in hav­ing set the bases upon which to con­strue a sys­tem­at­ic psy­cho­log­i­cal model of thought, re­quire (as in all of Freud) a co­her­ent re­de­f­i­n­i­tion with­in the cur­rent re­search con­text. Fol­low­ing the cog­ni­tive sci­ence per­spec­tive, we may over­come any du­al­ism so as to reach mul­ti­ple pro­cess­ing modes. In order to ac­count for ob­ser­va­tions in the clin­i­cal set­ting, as well as for all as­pects of in­for­ma­tion pro­cess­ing, in­clud­ing emo­tion­al in­for­ma­tion pro­cess­ing across the whole lifes­pan, here comes the the­o­ry of ‘mul­ti­ple codes’, which are three fun­da­men­tal modes, or three sys­tems by which human be­ings process in­for­ma­tion, in­clud­ing emo­tion­al in­for­ma­tion, and form in­ter­nal rep­re­sen­ta­tions: the sub-​symbolic non ver­bal mode, the sym­bol­ic non ver­bal mode and the sym­bol­ic ver­bal mode. 

Sub-​symbolic pro­cess­ing in­volves all those stim­uli - from feel­ings to motor and sen­so­ry in­for­ma­tion - non ver­bal, that are processed ‘in par­al­lel’: for ex­am­ple, when we un­der­stand the emo­tions in some­one’s fa­cial ex­pres­sion, or we com­pose a piece of music, or we rec­og­nize a fa­mil­iar voice in the com­mo­tion of a meet­ing, or we do a head­er on a cross at just the right time and height, or we in­tu­it the right tim­ing for in­ter­pre­ta­tion in psy­chother­a­py. Non ver­bal sym­bol­ic pro­cess­ing in­stead in­volves those men­tal im­ages (a face, a song, an ex­pres­sion, or as the Bea­t­les sang, some­thing in the way she moves at­tracts me like no other woman...), which, al­beit avail­able to con­scious­ness, can­not be trans­lat­ed into words. 

Fi­nal­ly, the ver­bal sym­bol­ic mode in­volves the re­fined men­tal in­stru­ment by which the in­di­vid­ual com­mu­ni­cates his in­ter­nal world to oth­ers. It is through this lat­ter mode that knowl­edge and cul­ture are passed on from one in­di­vid­ual to an­oth­er, and from one gen­er­a­tion to an­oth­er.  

The three sys­tems are gov­erned by dif­fer­ent prin­ci­ples, yet they are also in­ter­con­nect­ed. Our health de­pends on the rich­ness of their in­ter­con­nect­ed­ness. Bucci de­fines this com­plex and bidi­rec­tion­al con­nec­tion - from emo­tions to words and vice versa - as ‘ref­er­en­tial process’ , and she has elab­o­rat­ed tools to as­sess ref­er­en­tial ac­tiv­i­ty.

I be­lieve the ‘mul­ti­ple code’ model, al­though it tack­les the MBP bet­ter than oth­ers (in­deed, thanks to the sym­bol­ic and sub-​symbolic sys­tems we no longer speak of mind and body), ac­tu­al­ly con­tin­ues to face the prob­lem in the usual ut­ter­ly du­al­is­tic terms. In that, de­spite being a sug­ges­tive model, I high­ly doubt we may ex­haus­tive­ly solve the leap be­tween sym­bol­ic and sub-​symbolic in this way. The leap is the side ef­fect of our ways of writ­ing the ex­pe­ri­ence, not some­thing that ex­ists in it­self. 

Nonethe­less, we con­tin­ue to pose the prob­lem in the same way, re­lent­less­ly ask­ing our­selves whether mind and body are en­ti­ties that were once unit­ed into a sin­gle ob­ject, which West­ern thought then di­vid­ed (hence to be unit­ed again), or whether they are dis­tinct en­ti­ties to be kept sep­a­rate. 

We ask our­selves whether these en­ti­ties com­mu­ni­cate or not (how often do we hear that mind and body in­flu­ence each other, talk to each other!). Today many an­swer as fol­lows: of course they com­mu­ni­cate, through hor­mones! 

Kan­del, the Nobel prize for Med­i­cine, has proved it: en­vi­ron­ment, re­la­tion­ships and words exert a troph­ic in­flu­ence on the junc­tions (synaps­es) be­tween nerve cells.

Kan­del, neu­ro­sci­en­tist and Nobel for med­i­cine in 2000, has in fact mar­velous­ly shown that ex­pe­ri­ences be­come (also) bi­o­log­i­cal struc­tures. Thus, he has seem­ing­ly bridged the mind (en­vi­ron­ment) body gap. The con­tin­u­ous ac­ti­va­tion of cells at cer­tain neu­ronal junc­tions in fact trig­gers ge­net­ic cel­lu­lar mech­a­nisms that pro­mote the growth of fur­ther synaps­es in cor­re­spon­dence to those junc­tions. This means that ner­vous cells grow and con­nect thanks to synaps­es that are con­stant­ly ac­ti­vat­ed by en­vi­ron­men­tal in­puts. And the de­gree and pleas­ant­ness of the ac­tiv­i­ty have a troph­ic ef­fect in them­selves.  

It is in such a way that speech may pro­mote a pro­tein ex­pres­sion of genes, which, by in­flu­enc­ing the presy­nap­tic ionic canals, mod­i­fy the func­tion­al­i­ty of ner­vous areas in­volved, the num­ber and po­ten­cy of synaps­es. Speech, through the emo­tions it evokes, mod­i­fies the struc­ture and func­tions of ner­vous areas in­volved thanks to synap­tic plas­tic­i­ty. Fur­ther­more, when a bod­i­ly area changes, the whole body changes. 

Hence, re­main­ing at a bi­o­log­i­cal level, we might say that speech and trans­mit­ted af­fect in­flu­ence the body, just as cer­tain drugs do, faster in­deed, but less durably.  

All true, pro­vid­ed we re­main with­in the frame­work of the un­solv­able du­al­ism/monism, of causal­ism, or with­in a cer­tain logic. How­ev­er, upon step­ping into a dif­fer­ent logic, cer­tain re­marks arise

Per­son­al­ly, I feel dis­tant from those who rea­son along these lines, as­sert­ing that the mind body leap has now been solved, pre­cise­ly by cer­tain branch­es of med­i­cine such as psycho-​neuro-endocrinology. This sci­ence seems to have bridged the gap by prov­ing that the psy­che, the en­vi­ron­ment and bi­o­log­i­cal sys­tems/the body, in­flu­ence each other bidi­rec­tion­al­ly. And I also feel quite dis­tant from that rad­i­cal or elim­i­na­tive re­duc­tion­ism by which med­i­cine holds that the mind is the brain, while psy­cho­analy­sis holds that those who study the mind should not be con­cerned with the brain.  

Why? 

To an­swer, I shall at­tempt to step out­side the con­cep­tu­al perime­ter of du­al­ism/monism. 

And out­side the idea that mind and body are two truly ex­ist­ing things in the world. 

Let us start with ‘speech’, un­der­stood as some­thing ‘ab­stract‘ that in­flu­ences the ‘con­crete’, the junc­tions be­tween nerve cells.  

Why on earth should speech be some­thing ab­stract? 

Be­liev­ing speech is not an act of the body is an er­ro­neous thought, an ab­strac­tion that  whol­ly dis­re­gards its event and gives in to the su­per­sti­tion of mean­ing, for­get­ting that speech is in­stead born, is in­scribed and pro­ceeds from body to body, from bod­ies to bod­ies. 

Fol­low­ing Di­di­er Anzieu (and poets such as Rim­baud with Voyelles), speech has ma­teric and sen­si­ble qual­i­ties. Speech as sound is the mind’s first shell, from as far back as cer­tain ges­ta­tion pe­ri­ods of the fetus. Also, as cer­tain stud­ies have now con­firmed, fol­low­ing a sin­gu­lar, sub­jec­tive read (thus going be­yond a mere­ly bi­o­log­i­cal level), the out­comes of talk­ing cure have rich­er and more long-​lasting ef­fects than psy­chotrop­ic drugs alone. What is more, they oc­ca­sion­al­ly trig­ger more im­me­di­ate and stu­pe­fy­ing ef­fects: how many of us have ex­pe­ri­enced, as an­a­lysts and pa­tients, al­most mag­i­cal ther­a­peu­tic en­coun­ters, when after reach­ing the ses­sion an­guished and with stom­ach cramps, we leave rid of pain or anx­i­ety. 

This hap­pens be­cause words mod­i­fy what we call the body’s phys­i­ol­o­gy, and they do so not be­cause they are mag­i­cal, but for two fun­da­men­tal rea­sons. As we have said, be­cause they are events that are just as car­nal as the ac­tion of an arm or of a sen­so­r­i­al im­pres­sion (acoustic, motor, phona­to­ry, both in­ter­nal - when we utter a word or sound, and ex­ter­nal - when we lis­ten, be it even to our own speech as if sub­ject­ed to our own say­ing). There­fore, it is un­clear why words should not carry with them an op­er­a­tiv­i­ty that we are used to defin­ing con­crete or so­mat­ic. 

The sec­ond fun­da­men­tal rea­son is that it is just as un­clear why words should not be ther­a­peu­tic, name­ly, should not have to do with the truth of our suf­fer­ing. 

This all im­plies that this truth, far from lying in the ideal world of the spir­it, is al­ready arranged, al­lo­cat­ed, hav­ing the shape and na­ture of our car­nal pres­ence in the world, well be­fore and far deep­er than any non­sense we might tell our­selves.

Fol­low­ing the rea­son­ing of the philoso­pher Carlo Sini and of his pupil, philoso­pher and psy­cho­an­a­lyst An­drea Boc­chi­o­la, we re­al­ize that we never at­tempt to un­der­stand, in the only sen­si­ble way pos­si­ble (hence study­ing its gen­e­sis), the fact that there are bod­ies and minds in the world, and that they pos­sess a cer­tain shape and not an­oth­er. We never in­ves­ti­gate whether be­hind the words ‘body’ and ‘mind’ there truly are ‘things’, bear­ing an ex­is­tence that is in­de­pen­dent from the hori­zon of sense that des­ig­nates them. Vice versa, with­out re­flect­ing, we ad­here to the neu­ro­sciences or the psy­cholo­gies and/or to psy­cho­analy­sis, which take it as a given. Suf­fice it to think about how these dis­ci­plines’ sub­di­vi­sion it­self re­turns an an­thro­po­log­i­cal par­ti­tion where­by the bod­ies are as­signed to neu­ro­science (to med­i­cine), the minds to psy­chol­o­gy, and the psycho-​soma to psy­cho­analy­sis.    

 

Far from the idea of being able to dis­cov­er a ‘re­al­i­ty in it­self’, truly ex­ist­ing in the world, to in­stead at­tempt to un­der­stand the ge­neal­o­gy of our ways of trans­lat­ing ex­pe­ri­ence into the words of com­mon sense and sci­en­tif­ic the­o­ries, I be­lieve re­al­i­ty is some­thing that stems sole­ly from the en­counter with one an­oth­er, with our tools, our the­o­ret­i­cal mod­els, the spe­cif­ic lan­guage we are using. That is, with our prac­tices, which are ways of trans­lat­ing ex­pe­ri­ence. Out­side that en­counter, there is noth­ing. 

 

There­fore, a first (fun­da­men­tal) ques­tion aris­es: why is it that in the West this thought of ours is born, bear­ing di­vid­ing power, and why does this thought pose as its limit the mind and body to be unit­ed (if we are monists) into a sin­gle (un­think­able) ob­ject that is be­lieved to truly exist, a sort of in­di­vis­i­ble unit? 

And a sec­ond ques­tion: are we sure to be able to solve the mind body prob­lem re­main­ing with­in the logic of di­vid­ing thought and of re­al­i­ty con­ceived, in­deed, as in­di­vis­i­ble and on­to­log­i­cal­ly ex­ist­ing unit? 

 

Here some shall pace up and down: this is phi­los­o­phy! 

Yes, it is phi­los­o­phy. Or rather, thought. 

When we think, we no­tice that mind and body cease to be to­tal­ly dis­tinct ‘en­ti­ties’, to be kept dis­tinct, or (fol­low­ing the cur­rent trend) to be unit­ed into a hy­po­thet­i­cal, ab­surd, un­think­able, in­di­vis­i­ble sin­gle ob­ject. Mind and body thus be­come ‘things’ that are strict­ly con­tained in the words we use, so co­ex­is­tent as to both seem sur­faces that roll and un­roll, slip­ping into one an­oth­er with­out any so­lu­tion of con­ti­nu­ity. While we watch them dis­ap­pear from one side, there they ap­pear from the other. When we ob­serve them in this man­ner, it is al­most im­pos­si­ble to dis­tin­guish them: they cease to be on­to­log­i­cal­ly ex­is­tent things which, de­pend­ing on the trend, are de­fined as two sides of the same coin, or some­thing else, to then be­come move­ments, tra­jec­to­ries, con­tin­u­ous­ly end­ing up in one an­oth­er. We can no longer be­lieve that mind and body are di­vid­ed and to be unit­ed in search of cor­re­la­tions be­tween the two; nei­ther can we be­lieve that, were we to rea­son in terms of sys­tems (men­tal and bod­i­ly) the prob­lem would be solved. When we stop search­ing for cor­re­la­tions be­tween two en­ti­ties and come to terms with the fact that mind and body in­ces­sant­ly feed back to one an­oth­er, and that they ‘exist’ by the dif­fer­ence be­tween one an­oth­er, we un­der­stand what it is about: it is pre­cise­ly the (lin­guis­tic) trans­la­tion of the ex­pe­ri­ence we are liv­ing that cre­ates the two. The two are but writ­ing prac­tices; ways of writ­ing an ex­pe­ri­ence that in­volves the liv­ing, man; ways of con­tact­ing this data, man, on the part of man, and ways of at­tempt­ing to un­fold him and ex­plain him, through an al­pha­bet­i­cal de­vice and spe­cif­ic types of tools. 

A cer­tain cul­ture and a cer­tain lan­guage lead to be­lieve that what is real is only that which some­how falls under the five sens­es; it in­duces us to hal­lu­ci­nate a sort of pro­found split be­tween the so-​called ma­te­r­i­al world and ab­stract world. Let us think of ill­ness­es. They are phys­i­cal if they can some­how be seen and mea­sured, psy­chic if they are in­vis­i­ble. Yet this too is a bub­ble, a trap. The pres­ence or ab­sence of ‘mat­ter’ de­pends on the ‘level’ at which a ‘struc­ture’ or a ‘sys­tem’ are ob­served, and on the tools used to ob­serve them. It is there­fore al­ways a ques­tion of read. Nowa­days, for ex­am­ple, we can say that a cer­tain part of the autis­tic spec­trum is con­nect­ed to some­thing phys­i­cal, neurotransmitter-​related, be­cause we have equipped our­selves with high­ly so­phis­ti­cat­ed abil­i­ties in read­ing the so-​called world of neu­ro­trans­mit­ters, which we did not have be­fore. 

It is never a ques­tion of all-​or-nothing, phys­i­cal or psy­chic, con­crete or ab­stract! 

It is sim­ply an issue that can be con­tin­u­al­ly de­fined in feed­back from psy­chic to phys­i­cal and vice versa, phys­i­cal and psy­chic; micro and macro and vice versa, macro and micro. It de­pends on the ver­tex of ob­ser­va­tion, or bet­ter, on the mo­ment of scrolling of the Möbius strip in which we are sit­u­at­ed. This is clear­ly seen in the book (and in the col­lec­tion) Body and Sense (After Psy­cho­so­mat­ics)

The math­e­mati­cian Hof­s­tadter is right in as­sert­ing that we mark con­cep­tu­al bound­aries around en­ti­ties that we more eas­i­ly per­ceive, and in so doing, we tai­lor what re­al­i­ty seems to be to us. We are small mir­a­cles of self-​reference, be­liev­ing in things that dis­in­te­grate as soon as we set out to search for them, yet, when we do not search for them, they are ab­solute­ly real.

Gif

Being used to think­ing along the lines of more or less old reg­is­ters, in any case al­ways tra­di­tion­al, for ex­am­ple fol­low­ing du­al­ism of knowl­edge that pur­sues on­to­log­i­cal monism, we con­tin­ue to be­lieve in things like mind and body, in ab­solute ex­is­tence, di­vid­ed, and to be unit­ed. 

Our thought does not shift eas­i­ly, it does not re­flect deeply enough upon the fact that mind and body, in­ter­nal and ex­ter­nal, are not two ‘things’ in them­selves, real and pre-​constituted, which after the pre­sumed sep­a­ra­tion are to be unit­ed; fur­ther, how­ev­er one might carry out the sep­a­ra­tion, their in­di­vis­i­ble re­al­i­ty is sim­ply never reached.  

In the lit­er­a­ture we often find con­fu­sion: on one hand there is a dis­tinc­tion, bas­ing the psy­che on the body and vice versa, fol­low­ing a lin­ear path; there is then the at­tempt to rem­e­dy the dis­tinc­tion by con­ceiv­ing psy­che and body with­in a so-​called com­plex­i­ty (un­der­stood as a jum­ble of chaot­ic di­rec­tions and mul­ti­ple causal­i­ties, even retroac­tive); fi­nal­ly, we at­tempt to unite them into a sin­gle thing, ex­ist­ing in re­al­i­ty. Based on these premis­es, our thought seems to end­less­ly bounce be­tween du­alisms that are never fi­nite, which refer back to monisms that are never fi­nite, and vice versa, as in a mir­ror room.   

 

Freud him­self posed the prob­lem in a way that I would dare de­fine some­what in­gen­u­ous or aprob­lem­at­ic. In­deed, if we look close­ly, there is no in­ter­nal be­fore the or­gan­ism con­sti­tutes it­self as sen­tient thresh­old (that well-​known PC thresh­old -​perception/consciousness-​). ‘Ex­ter­nal phys­i­cal world’ and (in­ter­nal) ‘psy­chic ap­pa­ra­tus’ are psy­chic no­tions, prod­ucts, or for­ma­tions, in the sense of ob­jects that do not truly exist, or rather, that exist only in con­nec­tion to the lan­guage and con­cept. 

Thus, the prob­lem should be posed in an­oth­er way: where do we begin to ex­plain the psy­chic ap­pa­ra­tus (the in­ter­nal)? 

One might im­me­di­ate­ly ob­ject, as says Carlo Sini, that only mad­men con­fuse things with words.  

One might ob­ject that we all know that lan­guage al­ludes to re­al­i­ty! 

So put, this ob­jec­tion is not true and stems from the usual, con­tin­u­ous con­fu­sion: we con­fuse be­tween know­ing that, speak­ing, we al­lude to re­al­i­ty (truly ex­ist­ing out­side us), and know­ing what speech names as ‘re­al­i­ty’, name­ly, as what is other than speech, in­clud­ing the word ‘re­al­i­ty’.

When we begin to re­flect upon the fact that psy­cho­an­a­lyt­ic lan­guage is noth­ing more than trans­la­tion and in­ter­pre­ta­tion, and that things in them­selves there­fore do not truly exist, such as li­bido, dreams, oneir­ic self rep­re­sen­ta­tions, then con­tin­u­ing to be­lieve that lan­guage al­ludes to re­al­i­ty be­gins to come across as marked­ly pre-​logical, or in any case quite in­gen­u­ous. 

On the other hand, the exact sci­ences too (as they used to be de­fined) are imag­inif­ic lan­guages. Let us think of med­i­cine: it speaks of events (says the oc­cur­ring ex­pe­ri­ence) with­in its prac­tice of speech, that is, it mea­sures, con­strues and trans­lates into its signs, for ex­am­ple the signs of the elec­troen­cephalo­gram, the elec­tro­car­dio­gram, X-​Rays, CT scans, MRIs.

Why med­i­cine and psy­cho­analy­sis may tune into each other when read­ing ‘re­al­i­ty’ (Why being bilin­gual is use­ful):

Let us think of the phys­i­cal changes in para­dox­i­cal or REM sleep. For ex­am­ple, of the drop in mus­cle tone by which, when we dream, we can­not es­cape be­cause of motor im­po­tence. We can­not get out of bed and leave, un­less we sleep­walk. 

Let us now at­tempt to put to­geth­er the data re­fer­ring to sud­den mus­cle hy­po­to­nia (the drop in mus­cle tone) and those brief and re­cur­ring an­guished dreams in which we feel as if we were falling (as if we mys­te­ri­ous­ly slipped) from a sud­den step. And it truly feels as if we were tak­ing that leap, or we ac­tu­al­ly feel and see our­selves falling into the void of a precipice. 

At this stage, we may no longer sep­a­rate the fact read by med­i­cine and tra­di­tion­al­ly de­fined as phys­i­cal - the drop in mus­cle tone, with its sen­so­ry reper­cus­sions, trans­lat­ed into porto-​images -, that is, the uni­ver­sal data, from the sin­gu­lar data, name­ly, the spe­cif­ic con­tent of those oneir­ic an­guish­es, as might be read by psy­cho­analy­sis.  

I be­lieve that pro­vid­ing trans­fer­ence in­ter­pre­ta­tion in psy­cho­an­a­lyt­ic treat­ment to the ex­pe­ri­ence of fall-​loss that is present in the oneir­ic con­tent of the an­guished dream, with­out bear­ing in mind the ‘bi­o­log­i­cal’ data (the uni­ver­sal drop in mus­cle tone), is an ac­tion that is in­com­plete and in­cor­rect.  

Let us there­fore imag­ine putting the two pieces of data to­geth­er. 

How? 

Let us imag­ine that the sen­so­ry im­pres­sions as­so­ci­at­ed to the drop in mus­cle tone un­fold along a con­tin­u­um - hence they may not be sep­a­rat­ed - to­geth­er with the oneir­ic im­ages of fall-​leap, with their emotional-​affective loads, which dif­fer from dream­er to dream­er and from dream to dream. 

Or, let us imag­ine the ‘men­tal im­ages’ and their af­fec­tive loads as an ‘in­ter­nal­iz­ing’ of ‘bod­i­ly’ data (sen­so­ry im­pres­sions), and the bod­i­ly data as an ‘ex­ter­nal­iz­ing’ of the ‘men­tal’ ones. Let us thus at­tempt to fol­low them along these lines, these pieces of data, to think of them pre­cise­ly in their con­tin­u­ous reci­procity and spec­u­lar­i­ty, as if on a Möbius strip: as they in­ter­nal­ize, the sen­sa­tions as­so­ci­at­ed to the drop in mus­cle tone turn into af­fect and im­ages of fall/loss, which, as they ex­ter­nal­ize, once again turn into ‘phys­i­cal’ sen­sa­tions. 

As if on a strip that rolls and un­rolls, to then roll and un­roll again, con­tin­u­ous­ly.  

What might be the use in con­tin­u­ing to as­sert that a pathol­o­gy feeds on sole­ly ge­net­ic  (in­ter­nal) caus­es, or, sole­ly en­vi­ron­men­tal (ex­ter­nal)?  

Fur­ther­more: do ‘caus­es’ as we have al­ways un­der­stood them, truly exist? 

I shall bring the ex­am­ple of a very com­mon pathol­o­gy that is de­pres­sion, in psy­cho­an­a­lyt­ic tra­di­tion, an ill­ness that is ab­solute­ly ‘men­tal’, upon which schol­ars have been re­flect­ing for­ev­er. A book “La malat­tia in­glese” (The Eng­lish ill­ness, Si­mon­azzi L., 2004, Roma: Il Muli­no) tells about how a prob­lem that at the time was seen as only med­ical - melan­choly - was one of the most com­mon dis­or­ders in the British isles be­tween the 1500s and the 1700s, rais­ing such a de­bate as to in­volve not only doc­tors, but also the­ol­o­gists, schol­ars, philoso­phers and moral­ists. More­over, it dis­cuss­es the sci­en­tif­ic statute of med­i­cine and its re­la­tions to re­li­gion and magic, the re­la­tion be­tween soul and body, the func­tion of pas­sions, the pos­si­bil­i­ty of con­trol­ling them with rea­son, the re­la­tion be­tween civ­i­liza­tion process and ill­ness­es. 

Let us now try to imag­ine what hap­pens in the body read by med­i­cine, when one is de­pressed.  

A con­stant al­ter­ation of cer­tain neu­ropep­tides oc­curs, to­geth­er with the so-​called so­mato­form pains, or­gan­ic pains that are called ‘func­tion­al’. 

We are thus re­ferred back to a being to­geth­er of the state of de­pressed mood and func­tion­al pains with the al­tered me­tab­o­lism of those neu­ropep­tides.  

The big ques­tion: how is know­ing this use­ful? 

It is use­ful to psy­cho­an­a­lysts to move to­ward an ‘out-​of-sense’, be­cause the ‘neu­ropep­tide al­ter­ation’ issue at first glance rings no bells, it is dis­ori­ent­ing and not part of the jar­gon; hence, to at­tempt to ex­pand the feel­ing ‘with’, to at­tempt to ex­pand the un­der­stand­ing ‘with’, prac­tic­ing with this new dis­course and lin­guis­tic dou­bling helps to push one­self to­wards iden­ti­fi­ca­tions with that ill body that are ‘to the limit’. Unit­ing two lan­guages helps imag­ine that the ‘or­gan­ic’ pains that the pa­tient con­tin­ues to re­port run so in­ter­twined, in con­tin­u­ous re­versibil­i­ty, with an al­ter­ation of neu­ropep­tide mol­e­cules and elec­tric cir­cuits, that we are nat­u­ral­ly brought to push our­selves con­tin­u­ous­ly and si­mul­ta­ne­ous­ly, at least in our mute per­cep­tion, onto the car­nal­i­ty of sense and the sig­nif­i­cance of the body.  

Where a phe­nom­e­non is to be in­ter­pret­ed on the basis of trans­fer­ence and pa­tient his­to­ry, there is also, to­geth­er, in­sep­a­ra­bly, a meta­mor­pho­sis of the body, which is that same his­to­ry.  

Bear­ing all this in mind means being bilin­gual an­a­lysts. 

Of course, med­ical doc­tors would large­ly ben­e­fit too from work­ing side by side (at least) with psy­chol­o­gists! 

With this men­tal at­ti­tude, those who deal with the psy­che may de­vel­op a cu­rios­i­ty to­wards the fact that de­pres­sion means, si­mul­ta­ne­ous­ly, in­creased ac­tiv­i­ty of the sym­pa­thet­ic ner­vous sys­tem com­pared to the calm­ing parasym­pa­thet­ic one, thus, a re­duc­tion in time be­tween one heart­beat and an­oth­er, a re­duc­tion of vari­abil­i­ty in car­diac fre­quen­cy, with the con­se­quence that the heart can fall ill. Fur­ther­more, it means the re­lease of in­flam­ma­to­ry sub­stances, cy­tokines and c-​reactive pro­teins, the main in­flam­ma­to­ry blood index; it means in­crease in blood co­ag­u­la­tion, with the risk of throm­bo­sis; it means al­ter­ation of the sero­tonin cir­cuit, a poly­hedric mol­e­cule that is cru­cial for many func­tions in the body, with con­se­quent le­sion of blood ves­sel lin­ings, es­pe­cial­ly of the coro­nary ar­ter­ies. 

It is a grand the­atre of cor­por­e­ity, a metaphor­ic of the ex­tend­ed thing that also al­lows to dream of the pa­tient in an­oth­er way.

Call­ing the Un­con­scious - the Ex­ist­ing - does not sig­ni­fy re­triev­ing its statute of being, so in­con­sis­tent, so evanes­cent on an ontic level, it in­stead means re­triev­ing it on an eth­i­cal level. 

About it, Freud says: In any case, we must go there.  

In­deed, some­where, or rather, in an other space, nei­ther psy­chic nor or­gan­ic, the un­con­scious al­ways reap­pears, it can­not be elud­ed. It is not an order, it bears noth­ing of the sym­bol­ic order, we might de­fine it as the place of con­tin­u­ous mu­ta­tion, in­fin­i­ty, un­der­stood as limit, the his­to­ry of which in psy­cho­analy­sis I shall at­tempt to speak of next. 

Bion is the great analyst who at a certain point in life attempts to operate the change, breaking away from the idea that the Unconscious (its derivates) be reducible to knowing,  and introducing absolute doubt, the sign ‘O’: “[...] the facts “in themselves” [absolute] of the session. What the facts “in themselves” [absolute] are we shall never know; thus, I shall indicate them with the mark O”.

It is the Chilean Matte Blanco who identifies in infinity, tied to the unconscious, the very heart of psychoanalysis: Freudian dialectics between unconscious and consciousness becomes dialectics between the infinity of the unconscious and the finite limits of human consciousness. This conception of the unconscious entails surpassing a single ‘infinity’, and introduces the idea of infinities of a different order. The structural unconscious is essentially a symmetrical way of being (everything is evened out with the rest, no contradictions): “it is emotion and it is expressed as intensity, tending towards infinite values, even when on the surface it seems “tamed.

It is es­pe­cial­ly Lacan, at a cer­tain point, in his last sem­i­nars, who even­tu­al­ly al­ters the whole par­a­digm: the un­con­scious no longer func­tions as lan­guage, but it is that which does not func­tion at all, that which is not re­ducible to any know­ing.  

Cen­ter stage is no longer the sym­bol­ic but the Real, the im­pos­si­ble that can never be reached: it leaves traces, yet they are traces that “not only erase them­selves, but that any use of dis­course tends to erase, an­a­lyt­ic dis­course as any other”. 

In­deed, Lacan strong­ly un­der­lines that his Real has noth­ing to do with the bi­o­log­i­cal body, real, con­crete, so to speak, or with what is re­quired of re­al­i­ty in order to found sci­ence. I shall re­turn to a thought of his: “Who knows what hap­pens in one’s own body? It is some­thing ex­tra­or­di­nar­i­ly sub­jec­tive. For some it is even the sense they give to the un­con­scious...

The Un­con­scious is so­mat­ic process­es” says Freud in the Out­line of Psycho-​Analysis. Where ‘so­mat­ic process­es’ are un­der­stood as what is nei­ther writ­ten nor writable by any sci­ence, the uni­ver­sal, the sin­gu­lar ob­ject, the un­rep­re­sentable, the con­sti­tu­tive void with­out which there would be no thought. 

We think pre­cise­ly be­cause there ex­ists the un­think­able (the true Un­con­scious), that which may never be trans­lat­ed into rep­re­sen­ta­tion. The end, the (ul­ti­mate) truth, will never be reached. 

The Un­con­scious is not the Un­aware of the neu­ro­sciences! 

Let us sal­vage the speci­fici­ty of the sin­gle lan­guages, al­beit learn­ing to weave them to­geth­er, as we shall see in the clin­i­cal cases in the book. 

At this stage, what is the point of the term psy­cho­so­mat­ics, with all its vast world of re­search, the­o­ret­i­cal for­mu­la­tions and clin­i­cal ap­pli­ca­tions (which are in any case to be thor­ough­ly un­der­stood!), con­sid­er­ing this term and this world were born and raised with­in the mind body prob­lem and its con­cep­tu­al perime­ter? 

If the prob­lem is no longer that of bridg­ing the mind body gap, does it still make sense to speak of psy­cho­so­mat­ics

As we were saying, when we step outside the common thought that believes the internal (psyche) and the external (body) are pre-constituted ‘things’, and enter a chiasmatic logic, we understand that the deferred (indeed, external and internal) are not things at all, but rather that they emerge from language. Better still, they emerge from the threshold of language, from its crevice. Thus, they are articulated on the basis of their reciprocity and alternation, sharing the inside and the outside, internal and external, in the sense that one is clearly external for the other and vice versa. There exist neither origin nor destination; there exists oscillation, a vibration, a crossing, that does not occur from pre-constituted places, continuously leading the external (the environment and the body) to internalize itself (in the mind) and vice versa. Following this thought, external and internal cease to be res or substantia, to become movements imagined through our discourse

Now, in light of this thought, what is the mean­ing of data from the fa­mous sci­en­tif­ic stud­ies  that in­ves­ti­gate struc­tur­al and func­tion­al changes in­duced by re­la­tions (the en­vi­ron­ment, the ex­ter­nal) on the ner­vous sys­tem, in par­tic­u­lar and more gen­er­al­ly, on the neuro-​immuno-endocrine sys­tem, hence on the whole body (the in­ter­nal)? 

In short, what will it mean in the fu­ture to af­firm that the ex­ter­nal in­flu­ences the in­ter­nal

One might ob­ject: who re­nounces the ideal of ob­jec­tive know­ing, of dis­tinct things (in­ter­nal/ex­ter­nal; mind/body...), in a ‘per­fect­ly ob­jec­tive’ world? 

It is not a mat­ter of rid­ding our­selves of all sci­en­tif­ic re­search car­ried out until now, but of ask­ing our­selves: why, de­spite con­clu­sions, has the fig­ure of the re­searcher been re­pressed in con­tem­po­rary re­search? We are to find a way to rein­sert the sci­en­tist re­searcher into sci­ence, which would call into ques­tion the whole ma­te­ri­al­is­tic or meta­phys­i­cal per­spec­tive. A dif­fer­ent em­pha­sis should be posed on the sub­ject who ob­serves. The dif­fer­ences be­tween the meta­phys­i­cal and epis­te­mo­log­i­cal po­si­tions (which I would glad­ly call epis­te­mon­to­log­i­cal, bor­row­ing M. M. Ponty’s ‘new on­tol­ogy’) are enor­mous. 

These years, for ex­am­ple, have of­fered us a pletho­ra of stud­ies on the na­ture of con­scious­ness: in none of these stud­ies are cer­tain es­sen­tial fac­tors truly taken into ac­count, the vivid­ness, the in­trin­sic pres­ence of the sub­ject who per­ceives and car­ries out re­search, his the­o­ries, his moods, his in­escapable pro­jec­tions.  

The first step should be that of elab­o­rat­ing ex­per­i­men­tal prac­tices that may be ex­port­ed to other con­texts (for ex­am­ple, the prac­tices we are of­fered by mi­cro­physics?), with the chal­lenge of un­der­stand­ing how such prac­tices alter the na­ture of the de­bate on sci­en­tif­ic method, and ap­pre­ci­at­ing their role in a pos­si­ble sci­en­tif­ic rev­o­lu­tion. 

The in­for­ma­tion avail­able is al­ways nec­es­sar­i­ly in­com­plete, be­cause ul­ti­mate­ly, as we have seen, we may never reach the truth. At the basis of any know­ing there ex­ists the un­con­scious, un­der­stood as infi­nite ob­ject, limit, con­sti­tu­tive void, with­out which we would not be able to think, and hence we would not be able to carry out re­search. 

We are equipped with in­creas­ing­ly in­no­v­a­tive tools that allow us to per­ceive new im­ages and new the­o­ret­i­cal mod­els as we go on. We are con­stant­ly grap­pling with the un­der­stand­ing of ‘things’. Suf­fice it to think about the num­ber of statutes of the Un­con­scious that have been con­strued in time. 

We have at­tempt­ed to show how, in order to in­ter­pret things, we must end­less­ly start from the be­gin­ning, trace back his­to­ry, in an ef­fort that is first and fore­most ge­nealog­i­cal. 

Fur­ther­more, we must start think­ing in the un­usu­al terms of ‘things’ that are im­pos­si­ble to see, called in­ter­con­nec­tions, flows, dy­nam­ics, trans­for­ma­tions, move­ment. 

When we are able to do this, our un­der­stand­ing of sit­u­a­tions shifts. 

For ex­am­ple, the un­der­stand­ing of the con­cept of being ‘in­side’ (in­ter­nal) and ‘out­side’ (ex­ter­nal) shifts.  

It is dif­fi­cult to ex­plain in words that in­ter­nal and ex­ter­nal are only ever mov­ing sit­u­a­tions cre­at­ed by thought and lan­guage. Yet when we try to sup­port it with im­ages, in­deed imag­in­ing set­ting our­selves on a mov­ing bor­der as on a Möbius strip, which folds and un­folds and again folds in a tire­less move­ment, then ‘out­side’ and ‘in­side’, body and mind, ex­clu­sive­ly be­come ways of nam­ing a po­si­tion that hangs in the bal­ance of the spe­cif­ic mo­ment we are going through.   

We can­not stick any pole in the ground, hop­ing it might help us say: this is in­side, it is men­tal, and this is out­side, it is bod­i­ly, or this other is the en­vi­ron­ment.  

Only to then con­vince our­selves that we have fi­nal­ly bridged the gap be­tween the two (or the three). 

We may glimpse the end of this im­passe when we begin to feel and think in terms of a new ar­chi­tec­ture of being, sub­ject to con­tin­u­ous meta­mor­pho­sis and re­versibil­i­ty.  

Or­ga­niz­ing and con­sol­i­dat­ing are never al­lowed.  

Going back to Freud, from whom we start­ed, we must bear well in mind, al­ways, that there ex­ists no in­ter­nal be­fore the or­gan­ism has con­sti­tut­ed it­self as sen­tient thresh­old (the well-​known PC - per­cep­tion/con­scious­ness - thresh­old). ‘Ex­ter­nal phys­i­cal world’ and (in­ter­nal) ‘psy­chic ap­pa­ra­tus’ are there­fore ‘psy­chic’ no­tions, prod­ucts or for­ma­tions, in the sense of ob­jects that do not truly exist, or rather, that exist only in con­nec­tion to the lan­guage and con­cept. Thresh­old is a con­cept that al­ludes to some­thing that moves, a limit, an os­cil­lat­ing elas­tic sup­port that hosts and can­cels. 


[1] The essay was born with­in a long work­ing group at the Cen­tro Mi­lanese di Psi­coanal­isi Ce­sare Musat­ti. It avails it­self of Vi­viana Mari­bel Ram­pon’s con­tri­bu­tions (graph­ic and the part on ‘caus­es’), who is also re­spon­si­ble for all on­line or­ga­ni­za­tion­al as­pects. The edit­ing is by Clau­dio Cas­sar­do; the Eng­lish trans­la­tion is by Olivia March­ese.

Search:

Newsletter: